(i) [the student who deserves iti]j will get [the reward hej works for ]iIf iti is intended to be co-referential with the reward hej works for, and hej is intended to be co-referential with the student who deserves iti, and if the corefering terms are equated in the description, we have the paradox that a term a which properly contains a term b, is equal to a term b which is properly contained in a (the paradox being that a term must be both equal and unequal to another term). In the case of (i) the paradox is avoided if the description is something like (ii).
(ii) for all x, x:a student & for all y, y:a reward (if x works for y & x deserves y, then x will get y)LIT. Bach (1970, May (1985).
Back
PHONOLOGY: a °feature which characterizes sounds
that are produced by retracting the body of the tongue from its neutral position.
EXAMPLE: the vowel [u] differs from [i] in that [u] is characterized by [+back]
and [i] by [-back].
LIT.
Chomsky & Halle (1968),
Halle & Clements (1983).
Back-formation
MORPHOLOGY: a type of word formation by °analogy.
Back-formation occurs when speakers of a language assign a regular derivational structure
to a word, although a part of this structure, namely the
°base, did not previously exist. If this new base becomes a
word of the language, it is called a back-formation.
EXAMPLE: speakers of English have reinterpreted the
°primary compound baby-sitter as being
a °synthetic compound, i.e. consisting of the
base baby-sit and the suffix -er, and on the basis of this noun they
have coined the verb to baby-sit. Another example is self-destruct
from self-destruction. In the literature, the existence of back-formation is
taken as evidence for a °word-based
morphology.
LIT.
Marchand (1969),
Aronoff (1978),
Scalise (1984),
Booij (1989),
Spencer (1991).
Backward anaphora
°Cataphoric relation.
Bahuvrihi compound
MORPHOLOGY: a term first used by the Sanskrit grammarians to refer to a
particular type of compound, viz. compounds that lack a phonologically visible head,
and which predominantly refer to pejorative properties of human beings.
EXAMPLE: in regular compounds such as bloedneus 'bloody nose' the
word neus functions as the head, and bloed as the modifier which
attributes a property to the head neus, viz. that it is bleeding. Bahuvrihi
compounds like wijsneus 'wise guy' (lit. 'wise nose') or roodhuid
'redskin' (='Native American') are crucially different in this respect. A
wijsneus is not a nose that is wise. To distinguish between both types of
compounds, the term °endocentric compound
is used for compounds such as bloedneus, while the term bahuvrihi compound
or exocentric compound is used for compounds such as wijsneus.
LIT.
Kiparsky (1982),
Spencer (1991).
Bare plural
SEMANTICS: a plural noun phrase without an overt determiner.
EXAMPLE: cats, or pretty girls with blond hair are
bare plurals. Bare plurals can have a °generic
interpretation (in (i)) or an
°existential interpretation (in (ii)):
(i) Cats are intelligent (ii) Cats ruined my gardenCarlson (1977) analyzes bare plurals as basically names for kinds.
Barrier
SYNTAX: a category which serves as a blockade for
°government and/or
°movement. The original definition (from Chomsky 1986b)
is:
(i) g is a barrier for b iff (a) or (b): a. g immediately dominates d, d a °Blocking Category for b b. g is a Blocking Category for b, g =/= IPLIT. Chomsky (1986b), Rizzi (1990), Lasnik & Saito (1991).
Base
MORPHOLOGY: a term used for that part of a morphologically complex word to which
derivational or inflectional morphemes are added. EXAMPLE: in the English words
fatherhood and fathers, the noun father functions as the base
to which the derivational suffix -hood, and the inflectional suffix -s,
respectively, are added.
Base component
SYNTAX: Module of the grammar in which
°D-structures are generated by means of
°phrase structure rules
and the °Projection Principle, on the basis
of information from the °lexicon.
Base-generated
SYNTAX: Whatever is an element of a
°D-structure, hence is generated by the
°base component.
Belief sentence
°Propositional attitude.
Bijection Principle
SYNTAX: principle stating that an °operator
must bind one and only one °variable, where 'variable'
is defined as any locally A-bar bound NP. This principle (BP for short) accounts for
weak °crossover violations, as in (i):
(i) *Whoi does hisi mother admire ti (ii) Whoi ti likes hisi motherThe BP excludes the interpretation for (i) where both his and ti are °bound variables bound by the °operator who. The bound variable reading for the pronoun is allowed in (ii), since the pronoun, being locally A-bound by the trace of who, does not syntactically count as a variable.
Bilabial
PHONOLOGY: bilabial sounds are produced by using both lips.
EXAMPLE: in English /p/, /b/ and /m/ are bilabial.
Binary Branching Constraint
MORPHOLOGY: a constraint on concatenative
°word formation which says that in the process
of word formation only two morphemes can be concatenated at the same time. Hence, the
compound a,b,c either has the structure [[[a] [b]] [c]], or
the structure [[a] [[b] [c]]], but not the ternary structure [[a] [b] [c]].
°Circumfixes are problematic with respect to this
constraint.
SYNTAX: Constraint proposed in
Kayne (1984) which rules out syntactic
structures in which a phrase contains more than two immediate constituents
(i.e. no °node in a °tree may
have more than two branches).
LIT.
Spencer (1991).
Binary connective
°Connective.
Bind
SYNTAX: In °Binding
Theory, a relation that may obtain between two (NP-)nodes. In Chomsky
(1981), a node a is said to bind a node b iff a
and b have the same
°referential index
(°coindexing), and a
°c-commands b. Furthermore, if a
binds b then, if a is in an
°A-position, a A-binds b and if
a is in an A-bar position, a A-bar-binds b. These notions
are crucially involved in the definition of the conditions of the Binding Theory.
LIT.
Chomsky (1981,
1982).
Binding
SYNTAX: A relation in which the reference of a certain element is dependent
on the reference of another element. See °Binding
theory, °Bind.
SEMANTICS: The relation obtaining between a
°quantifier All(v) or Exists(v) and
the occurrences of the °variable v in its
°scope:
(i) All(v)[ ... v ... ] (ii) Exists(v)[ ... v ... ]In the following formula only the first occurrence of x is bound by All but not the second (which is not in the °scope of All):
(iii) All(x)[P(x) -> Q(y)] & R(x)The first occurrence of x is called a bound variable, the second occurrence is called a free variable.
Binding Condition
SYNTAX: condition on the binding possibilities of certain elements. Also called
'Binding Principle'. See °Binding theory.
Binding Domain
SYNTAX: domain in which an °anaphor or
°pronoun must or must not be bound. In one version
elaborated as the °Minimal Governing
Category. Also called local domain. See °Binding
theory.
Binding theory
SYNTAX: theory on °A-binding. Binding theory
consists of the three conditions in (i).
(i) A An °anaphor is °bound in its °binding domain B A °pronominal is °free in its binding domain C An °R-expression is freeUsually these conditions are conjoined with a characterization of anaphors, pronominals and R-expressions in terms of the features 'anaphor' and 'pronominal' as in (ii).
(ii) a [+anaphor, -pronominal] = anaphor b [+anaphor, +pronominal] = °PRO c [-anaphor, -pronominal] = R-expression d [-anaphor, +pronominal] = pronominalLIT. Chomsky (1981, 1986a).
Biuniqueness
MORPHOLOGY: an axiom explicitly assumed within the framework of
°Natural Morphology which entails that every
morpheme has one phonological form and one meaning, and every meaning (or grammatical
category) corresponds to exactly one phonological form.
LIT.
Dressler (1985a,
1985b).
Bleeding order
PHONOLOGY: order of rules such that one rule destroys the input of another rule.
EXAMPLE: consider the following two rules proposed by Schane (1968) for French:
(a) a vowel is °nasalized before a
°nasal, and (b) a nasal is dropped in syllable-final
position. To derive the output [bõ] of bon, rule (a) must be applied
before rule (b). If (b) would precede rule (a) it would bleed rule (a): the vowel
cannot be nasalized anymore and [bõ] could not be derived.
Blending
MORPHOLOGY: an improductive type of word formation by which a new word is
formed out of the initial phoneme(s) of one word and the final phoneme(s) of another.
It is often argued that this type of word-formation does not belong to the I-language.
EXAMPLE: A prototypical example of a blend is the English word smog
which is a merger of the words smoke and fog.
Blocking
MORPHOLOGY: a process assumed in Aronoff (1976) by which the existence of
one word, which is listed in the mental dictionary, prevents the application of an
unproductive word formation rule, if that application would give rise to a complex
word having the same semantics as the already existing word.
EXAMPLE: the English suffixes -ity and -ness are very
similar, the difference being that the former is unproductive and the latter productive.
Both can be added to adjectives ending in -ous (curious:
curiosity: curiousness). However, if there already exists a noun
corresponding to the adjective in -ous as in the case of glorious:
glory, gracious: grace, furious: fury
etc., -ity affixation, but not -ness affixation, is disallowed:
*gloriosity: gloriousness, *graciosity: graciousness,
and *furiosity: furiousness.
LIT.
Aronoff (1976),
Spencer (1991).
Blocking Category
SYNTAX: g is a Blocking Category (BC) for b iff g
is not °L-marked and g
°dominates b. The concept of a BC plays a
role in the definition of °barrierhood, introduced in
Chomsky (1986b).
Bound morpheme
MORPHOLOGY: a morphological element that can only appear as a proper subpart
of a word, i.e. an element which cannot function as an independent word or
°free morpheme. One can distinguish two types of
bound morphemes: (a) °affixes, and (b)
°roots.
EXAMPLE: the English word agreement contains the nominalizing affix
-ment which is not a word in its own right, and therefore -ment is
an affix of English. A form like mit (as in permit, remit,
commit, etc.) which is not an affix but a root (since affixes may attach to
it, forming a word) also cannot occur freely in syntax and may be called a bound
morpheme for this reason.
Bound variable
°Variable,
°binding.
Boundary
MORPHOLOGY: a formal device used in
Chomsky & Halle (1968)
to express a distinction between two types of affixes. The assumption that affixes
are associated with different boundaries, viz. '+' (morpheme boundary) and '#'
(word boundary) accounts for the fact that the English suffixes -ity
and -ness behave differently with respect to a number of phonological
rules, as shown in (i):
(i) prodúctive productívity prodúctiveness op[ei]que op[æ]city op[ei]quenessThe claim is that -ity is a morpheme-boundary or formative-boundary affix (i.e. +ity), and -ness a word-boundary affix (i.e. #ness). °Level Ordering Hypothesis.
Bounded/unbounded
PHONOLOGY: one of the hypothesized typological parameters that define stress
systems
(cf. Hayes 1981)). It fixes the number
of syllables of a
°foot as either two (bounded) or indefinite (unbounded or
n-ary).
Bounding node
SYNTAX: node that plays a role in determining whether a movement is local
enough; °Bounding theory. Traditionally,
NP and S (in English) or S' (in Italian) are considered bounding nodes. More recently,
bounding nodes have been defined in terms of °barriers.
LIT.
Chomsky (1981,
1986b),
Rizzi (1982).
Bounding theory
SYNTAX: Theory about the locality of movement. The main principle of Bounding
theory is the Subjacency condition, which forbids movement across more than one
°bounding node.
EXAMPLE: in (i) which books has been moved over two bounding nodes,
NP and CP. In (ii), NP and IP are the relevant bounding nodes. In (i) the so-called
°Complex NP Constraint is violated, in
(ii) the so-called °Subject Condition. Thus,
the Subjacency condition subsumes both the Complex NP Constraint and the Subject
Condition.
(i) *which booki did John meet [NP a child [CP who read ti]] (ii) *the man [CP whoi [IP [NP pictures of ti] are on the table]]]LIT. Chomsky (1965, 1981, 1986b), Lasnik & Saito (1984, 1991), and (contra Subjacency), Bresnan (1973, 1976), Brame (1978).
Braces
PHONOLOGY: a notational convention to abbreviate two rules.
EXAMPLE: braces are a means to abbreviate the rules (i) and (ii) as the
one rule (iii).
(i) A -> B / C _____ (ii) A -> B / D _____ C (iii) A -> B / { } ______ DLIT. Chomsky & Halle (1968).
Bracket Erasure Convention
PHONOLOGY/MORPHOLOGY: a convention proposed in Kiparsky (1982) stating that
internal brackets are erased at the end of a lexical level or
°stratum. As a consequence of this
convention words become phonologically inert at the end of each lexical level, i.e.,
they can no longer be affected by cyclic phonological rules. After bracket erasure,
morphologically derived words are treated as though they were underived. In Kiparsky's
view this inertness extends to morphological processes, and word formation rules therefore
do not have access to the internal structure of words derived at an earlier level.
Thus, Siegel's (1977)
°Adjacency Condition
or Williams' (1981a)
°Atom Condition can be reduced
to the Bracket Erasure Convention.
LIT.
Kiparsky (1982,
1985),
Spencer (1991).
Bracketed grids
PHONOLOGY: a representation of stress combining aspects of grid (only) theory
and °tree (only) theory. Grids as hierarchically layered
structures are provided with constituent information indicated by parentheses.
EXAMPLE: The stress pattern of the English word apalachicola can be
represented by the following bracketed grid
(cf. Halle & Vergnaud 1987):
* * * (* .)(* .)(* .) apa lachi colaLIT. Hammond (1984), Hayes (1987)), Halle & Vergnaud (1987), Kager (1989), Hayes (1991).
Bracketing paradox
MORPHOLOGY: a situation in which the morphological structure of a word which
one would like to propose for semantic reasons does not correspond to the structure
one would like to propose for phonological reasons. Hence, a situation in which
morphophonological structure and semantic structure are not isomorphic. The existence
of bracketing paradoxes is closely associated with level-ordering theories such as
Pesetsky's (1979) and Kiparsky's (1982)
theories of
°Lexical Phonology/Morphology. These theories
propose morphological structures on the basis of level-ordering which are sometimes in
conflict with the semantic structure.
EXAMPLE: A well-known case of a bracketing paradox independent of
°level-ordering involves the English
comparative suffix -er and the negative prefix un-. In this case
the phonological argumentation derives from a well-known restriction on the attachment
of -er. This suffix is subject to the following constraint: it may attach to
monosyllabic adjectives, and a small class of bisyllabic ones with a light final
syllable, while it may not attach to adjectives with two or more heavy syllables.
Compare (i):
(i) blacker, softer, poorer, nicer happier, luckier, heavier *directer, *complexer, *eloquenter, *importanterApparent counterexamples involve the prefix un-, since unhappier, unluckier etc. are well-formed. In level-ordering theories, the solution to this problem is almost trivial: assume that -er suffixation takes place at an earlier level than un- prefixation. In that case -er is added to, for instance, happy, and un- to happier, and the phonological condition on -er suffixation is not violated. Hence, the phonologically motivated morphological structure of unhappier is [un [happy-er]A]A. However, this structure raises a problem of interpretation. The meaning of the word unhappier can be paraphrased as 'more not happy', i.e. with more having °scope over un-, and crucially not as 'not more happy' with reversed scope. Hence the semantically motivated structure is [[un happy] er].
Bridge verb
SYNTAX: verb which allows wh-movement out of its complement. As
shown by the grammaticality judgments in (i), say and think are
examples of bridge verbs, while quip and whisper are not.
(i) whoi did you think/say/*quip/*whisper [ that Bill saw ti ]
By-phrase
SYNTAX: optional °adjunct in a
°passive construction headed by by
and containing the logical subject.
EXAMPLE: in (i) the by-phrase contains Vitesse, the logical
subject of won (cf. Vitesse won the match). As shown by (ii),
by-phrases are also possible in nominal passives.
(i) The match was won by Vitesse (ii) Carthago's destruction by the Romans (iii) They kissed by the light of the moonNot all languages allow passive by-phrases, and not all phrases headed by by are passive by-phrases: in (iii) the light of the moon is not interpreted as an argument of the verb kiss.