Object control verb
SYNTAX: verb which selects both an NP and an infinitival clause as its
complements and requires that the NP must °control
the °PRO subject of the infinitival complement.
EXAMPLE: A standard example is (i):
(i) Johnj persuaded Susani [PROi/*j to leave]
Object language
°Meta language.
Oblique case
SYNTAX: non-canonical case which is assigned by a governing preposition. See
°Of-insertion and
°inherent case.
LIT.
Chomsky (1981).
Oblique context
°Opaque context.
Obstruent
PHONOLOGY: a name referring to consonants
(°stops and
°fricatives) that are produced by narrowing or
complete closure of the vocal tract; the air pressure is built up inside the cavity.
Obstruents share the feature [-son].
Of-insertion
SYNTAX: insertion of the preposition of in order to case-mark a NP object
of a noun:
(i) [NP the destruction of [NP the city]]The case assigned by the preposition of is generally referred to as oblique case.
One-affix-one-rule hypothesis
MORPHOLOGY: a hypothesis which says that a word formation rule specifies a
unique phonological operation which is performed on the base. It specifies a unique
syntactic label and subcategorization frame, as well as a unique semantic reading.
EXAMPLE: in English the suffix -er is used to form agent nouns
(worker, player) as well as comparatives (quicker, happier). Given
the differences in syntactic category and meaning, the one-affix-one-rule hypothesis
entails that there are two -er affixation rules. This example seems rather
trivial, but things become less trivial if one knows that -er is also used
to form instrument nouns (hanger, glider) since the circumstances under which
agent nouns and instrument nouns are formed are identical. The one-affix-one-rule
hypothesis says that we have two rules here, but this claim does not explain the
similarities between both rules of -er nominalization.
LIT.
Scalise (1984).
Opacity Condition
SYNTAX: condition which is in effect a reinterpretation of the
°Specified Subject Condition
as a condition on anaphors (including NP-traces). It is the precursor of
°condition A of the
°binding theory. The Opacity Condition
states that
(i) An anaphor cannot be °free in the domain of the subject of beta, beta minimal (beta = NP, S')LIT. Chomsky (1980).
Opaque context
SEMANTICS: a context in which substitution of two expressions with the same
°reference salva veritate (i.e. keeping the
°truth value constant) is impossible. The verb
hope in (ia) creates an opaque context. This can be seen by replacing the murderer
of Smith with an expression which is assumed to have the same reference (in this
case John's brother). The result in (ic) is a sentence which does not need to
have the same truth value as (ia). One has only to imagine a situation in which
John does not know that his beloved brother is the unknown murderer.
(i) a John hopes that the murderer of Smith will be hanged b The murderer of Smith is his (i.e. John's) brother c John hopes that his brother will be hangedMany different elements may create opaque contexts: words that express propositional attitudes (like know and believe, fear and hate), verbs like seek and want, temporal and modal expressions. Contexts of direct and indirect speech may also be opaque contexts. The pattern in (i) can be explained by making a distinction between °extension and °intension. The murderer of Smith and John's brother may have the same extension in a particular situation, but their intension, their meaning is different. A context which not opaque is called a transparent context.
Open syllable
PHONOLOGY: a name for a °syllable without
a consonant at the end: C0 V or C0 VV.
EXAMPLE: all syllables in the English name A-me-ri-ca are open.
Operator
SYNTAX: element, such as a °quantifier
(i) or a °wh-phrase (ii), which
°A-bar binds a
°variable (x) at °LF:
(i) Mary loves someone LF: someone x, Mary loves x (ii) Who does Mary love? LF: for which x, Mary loves xLIT. Chomsky (1981).
Operator binding
SYNTAX: Binding by an °operator.
LIT.
Chomsky (1981).
Oral
PHONOLOGY: oral sounds are produced without a lowered velum, so that the
air can escape through the mouth, not through the nose (cf.
°nasal).
Ordering hypothesis
°Level ordering Hypothesis.
Overgeneration
MORPHOLOGY: a property of (word formation) rules which entails that they
are able to generate entities which are fully well-formed, but are not attested or
used by the native speakers of a language.
EXAMPLE: if the rule for -al affixation may derive attested forms
such as arrival and refusal as well as unattested forms such as
derival and describal, we say that the -al rule overgenerates.
LIT.
Halle (1973),
Spencer (1991).
Overlapping reference
SYNTAX: situation where two referring expressions are not
°disjoint in reference, but not strictly
°coreferential either.
EXAMPLE: the ill-formed idiom *we lost my way requires we
and my to be strictly co-referential (cf. I lost my way), but
we and my only have an overlapping reference, hence the
ill-formedness.
LIT.
Chomsky (1981),
Lasnik (1989).
Overt syntax
SYNTAX: (minimalist theory) syntax before Spell-out.
LIT.
Chomsky (1992).